English, asked by mousum27, 5 months ago

Find the homographs with the help of the given clues.
1 .to prevent from being seen
skin of a large animal
2. a small sticker on a letter
to bring down your foot hard
3. a group with members
one of the four suits of cards
4. to teach
a mode of transport
5. a long-legged bird
to stretch your neck to see something

6.a game ending in a tie
to attract or pull​

Answers

Answered by AGRAWALGRACY77
2

Explanation:

onsensus was achieved. The semantic categories were

chosen to reflect the origin of each meaning of the word

according to The American Heritage Dictionary of the

American Language (1973). When two closely related

meanings came from the same source, they were treated

as a single category. If separating two related meanings

resulted in many responses that could be assigned to

either category, the meanings were combined (e.g., the

"dollar" and "statement" meanings of BILL were

combined under "money"). Words referring to both

an object and an action involving that object (e.g.,

DRILL) were included in a single category. Furthermore,

a response that could not be classified with any of the

meanings or that was obviously the result of a misin-

terpretation was placed in the questionable (?) category.

Finally, since all responses are reported, anyone wishing

to disagree with our judgments can readily recategorize

the items.

Pearson product-moment correlations were calcu-

lated between our norms and others using homographs

common to both sets. Before calculating these correla-

tions, meanings were combined when necessary to

coordinate categories in different norms, and, there-

fore, these correlations need to be regarded as approxi-

mate. All values were converted to proportions and

averaged over sex of the subject. Both dominant and

nondominant meanings were included. Thus 106 pairs of

HOMOGRAPH NORMS 17

proportions were used in calculating the r with the

Cramer (1970) norms. Similarly, 60, 83,186, and 148

pairings were used in calculating the respective rs for the

Geis and Winograd (1974), Kausler and Kollasch (1970),

Perfetti et al. (1971), and Wollen et al. (1980) norms.

The results of this correlational analysis are shown in

Table 1. All of the values appear to be fairly high and

agree quite well with those reported by Wollen et al.

(I980). This agreement is obtained despite differences in

subjects, sample size, evaluating judges, geographic

region, and, finally, despite differences in procedure.

Our norms and those of Cramer (1970) both employed

the "first response" technique and correlated .79.

Wollen et al. used short phrases and careful control of

the timing arrangements, and, despite these differences,

our values correlated .79 with theirs. Apparently,

greater control does not substantially alter the pro-

portions of responses allocated to the various meanings.

Table 2 presents each homograph listed in alphabetical

order. The meanings, or senses, of each item are listed

directly under it, and, in parentheses, the number of

subjects providing responses with that meaning is given.

As indicated, the ? category was used only when no

other possible meaning could be determined. The N/R

category represents "no responses," or omissions.

Note that the individual responses (and their frequencies)

are shown to the right of each meaning.

Similar questions