Science, asked by anweshanaik51, 1 month ago

which is the most representative of dogs vision? A red- green colour blind. B Black white vision.​

Answers

Answered by MissCornetto
2

Explanation:

which is the most representative of dogs vision?

= A red- green colour blind

Answered by Jaivardhan1991
0

Answer:

Neurobiological and molecular studies suggest a dichromatic colour vision in canine species, which appears to be similar to that of human red–green colour blindness. Here, we show that dogs exhibit a behavioural response similar to that of red–green blind human subjects when tested with a modified version of a test commonly used for the diagnosis of human deuteranopia (i.e. the Ishihara's test). Besides contributing to increasing the knowledge about the perceptual ability of dogs, the present work describes for the first time, to our knowledge, a method that can be used to assess colour vision in the animal kingdom.

Explanation:

Dogs' retinal structure clearly provides the potential for colour vision [1,2]. Specifically, visual-evoked potential [3,4] and immunohistochemical [1] studies have demonstrated that dogs possess two classes of cone pigments, one sensitive to long/medium wavelength light (555 nm spectral sensitivity; red/green) and the other sensitive to short wavelength light (429 nm spectral sensitivity; blue). The presence of these two discrete cone subtypes indicates a potential dichromatic vision. Concerning visual acuity, dogs are less able than humans to perceive clearly all the details of an object (four to eight time worse than humans) [5,6]. This is owing to the different neural structures of the dogs' eyes and in particular to the fewer connections of the rods to the ganglion cells and the smaller number of optic nerve fibres [5]. Furthermore, dogs can discriminate brightness differences but their ability is about two times worse than in humans [7].

Although early behavioural studies on dogs' colour vision produced conflicting results (reviewed by [8]), recent behavioural studies support the presence of dichromatic vision in canine species, indicating that colour cues are important for dogs during their normal activities under natural photonic lighting conditions [9].

Colour vision tests in the animal kingdom include both spontaneous and learned behaviour [10]. However, the most employed technique of testing colour vision in dogs uses associative learning with a food reward [9,10,11]. Using this procedure, Kasparson et al. [9] recently showed that colour proved to be more informative than brightness when dogs choose between visual stimuli differing both in brightness and hue. Associative learning was also used by Neitz et al. [11] to study different wavelength colour matching in three adult pure breed dogs. Overall, results of this work supported the hypothesis that colour perception is essential for canine vision and that it is dichromatic in character. In addition, computer estimation of the spectral sensitivity of the two photopigments of the dog's retina suggested that dichromatic vision in canine species resembles that of human deuteranopia (i.e. red–green colour blindness).

In order to directly test this hypothesis, we used to our knowledge, for the first time, an orienting response (e.g. movements of the eyes, the head and the whole body) to movements of a coloured target in the dog's visual field. The employment of unlearned response has a clear advantage because no preliminary training is required prior to the colour vision test, allowing the testing of a large number of subjects in a short period of time and avoiding motivational and reinforcement issues typical of learned response (e.g. food reward occurring during associative learning).

Investigating the understanding of colour perception/blindness in dogs is particularly interesting for two main reasons: (i) the dog is an important animal model of human retinal genetic disorders [1]; and (ii) the dog plays a number of significant roles within the human community (e.g. animal assisted therapy, search and rescue work and as guide dogs for visually-impaired humans) often requiring the use of visual cues.

Furthermore, considering that dogs' vision is weaker than the human one, this could affect their responses in an ethological experiment [6], deepening the understanding of colour perception could be decisive in the design of visual tasks suitable for dogs' visual capabilities.

Subjects were 21 domestic dogs of various breeds. We excluded six dogs: four dogs, because after hearing the beep used to capture their attention on the screen, they did not look at any stimuli; two dogs were potentially influenced by the owner during the test (i.e. the owner repositioned the dog's head to the screen). Hence the final sample consisted of 16 dogs (three Australian shepherds, one Épagneul Breton, one Weimaraner, one Labrador retriever and 10 mixed-breed dogs). Dogs ranged from 1 to 8 years of age (2.00 ± 1.96; mean years ± s.d). All dogs (nine females and seven males) were pets living in households. Only one male and five females were desexed. No subject had been tested previously.

Similar questions